Tuesday, May 1, 2012

"Delusions are... now conceptualized as dimensional entities rather than categorical ones, lying at the extreme end of a "belief continuum"... A truly comprehensive model of the persecutory delusion requires further elucidation at the neurochemical and genetic levels." - PsychiatryOnline | American Journal of Psychiatry | Cognitive Neuropsychiatric Models of Persecutory Delusions

"Delusions are... now conceptualized as dimensional entities rather than categorical ones, lying at the extreme end of a "belief continuum"...
A truly comprehensive model of the persecutory delusion requires further elucidation at the neurochemical and genetic levels.
Finally, genetic studies, in the absence of a definitively linked genomic region of, for example, schizophrenia R1584BABBEHIC, currently are being designed to reduce genetic heterogeneity at the entry point of proband ascertainment. Thus, dimensional variables (e.g., delusional ideation, social cognitive skills) or symptom clusters (e.g., reality distortion) become intermediate phenotypes worthy of study (for preliminary examples of this approach, see references R1584BABJIDCAR1584BABEFBIB). If the relevant social cognitive mechanisms can be reliably characterized and shown to be heritable, this approach could lead to more definitive linkage results and to the elucidation of the genetic background of the illnesses in which persecutory delusions arise."

PsychiatryOnline | American Journal of Psychiatry | Cognitive Neuropsychiatric Models of Persecutory Delusions

The American Journal of Psychiatry, VOL. 158, No. 4

1
Reviews and Overviews |
Cognitive Neuropsychiatric Models of Persecutory Delusions
Nigel J. Blackwood, M.A., M.R.C.Psych.; Robert J. Howard, M.D., M.R.C.Psych.; Richard P. Bentall, Ph.D.; Robin M. Murray, F.R.C.Psych., D.Sc.
Am J Psychiatry 2001;158:527-539. 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.4.527

Abstract
OBJECTIVE: The major cognitive theories of persecutory delusion formation and maintenance are critically examined in this article. METHOD: The authors present a comprehensive review of the literature, citing results of relevant functional neuroimaging and neural network studies. RESULTS: People with persecutory delusions selectively attend to threatening information, jump to conclusions on the basis of insufficient information, attribute negative events to external personal causes, and have difficulty in envisaging others’ intentions, motivations, or states of mind. Presence of the "reality distortion" cluster of psychotic symptoms correlates with cerebral blood flow in the left lateral prefrontal cortex, ventral striatum, superior temporal gyrus, and parahippocampal region. Social cognitive processing (selective attention to threat, attribution of causation or mental states) in normal subjects involves similar areas. Neural network models of persecutory delusions highlight the importance of disordered neuromodulation in their formation and of disordered neuroplasticity in their maintenance. CONCLUSIONS: Further studies examining the interaction of these cognitive processes, cross-sectionally and longitudinally, at cognitive psychological, neural network, and functional neuroanatomical levels are warranted to establish a comprehensive cognitive neuropsychiatric model of the persecutory delusion.Abstract Teaser
Figures in this Article

A cognitive neuropsychiatric model of the persecutory delusion specifies the cognitive abnormalities underlying delusion formation and maintenance, and in turn it characterizes these processes in functional neuroanatomical terms R1584BABIHDIIR1584BABJDBJG. In this paper we review the major cognitive theories currently proposed as explanatory frameworks for the genesis and maintenance of persecutory delusions, together with relevant neural network and functional neuroimaging studies. There is a significant clinical need for a satisfactory explanatory model: persecutory delusions are preoccupying, obtrusive, and distressing R1584BABGGHIC and stand out as directly related to serious violent crime R1584BABCGIDIR1584BABHAFDC. Patients act on their persecutory delusional beliefs R1584BABHFEJI, R1584BABHIHFB, particularly when the belief has a significant affective impact R1584BABDGHIG, R1584BABBECEJ. A plausible model has the potential to guide future research in both cognitive behavior and pharmacotherapeutic approaches to the treatment of delusions.
Classical definitions of delusions maintained that they were qualitatively different from normal beliefs R1584BABICDGD. They were operationally defined as false beliefs held with unusual conviction, whose absurdity was manifest to others and which were not amenable to logic R1584BABFCIDC. However, this monolithic definition has been modified in the light of more recent phenomenological studies. Delusions are not incorrigible: delusional conviction, preoccupation, and distress fluctuate over time R1584BABFFIGH, R1584BABEFHIB and can be reduced by specific cognitive behavior interventions R1584BABDBIGHR1584BABDBBCB. Delusions are therefore now conceptualized as dimensional entities rather than categorical ones, lying at the extreme end of a "belief continuum" R1584BABHEEGAR1584BABFHCDH.
Earlier explanatory frameworks related persecutory delusions to disordered intelligence, thought, affect, or perception. They had obvious limitations and lacked experimental justification R1584BABHFIEC. Equally, simplistic models that explained persecutory delusions in terms of neuropsychological dysfunction were discredited by studies in which no specific neuropsychological deficit, e.g., in mnemonic R1584BABCFJAD, R1584BABHBCEF or executive R1584BABECIFI, R1584BABCCHBJ function, consistently correlated with measures of persecutory delusions R1584BABCEFICR1584BABBJAJD.
Cognitive psychological formulations noted that persecutory delusions often emerged as misinterpretations of social interactions and events rather than of neutral or impersonal events R1584BABGDFEI: people with persecutory delusions are preoccupied with the intentions of others. Persecutory delusions may thus represent a disturbance of the psychological processes mediating the formation and maintenance of normal social beliefs.
Normal formation of social beliefs depends on 1) selection of social data in the environment relevant to oneself and 2) inferential processing using these data to determine others’ intentions toward oneself in order to guide one’s own behavior. Such social inferential processing is based on access to information stored in the "self-schema," a repository of autobiographical experience that includes information about the self, relationships with others, and social situations, which determines what is encoded and how it is interpreted R1584BABHJEIJ. Biases or deficits in these processes (attentional bias, attributional bias, a jumping-to-conclusions reasoning bias, and theory of mind deficits) have been proposed to have etiological significance in the formation of persecutory delusions (F1). These will be considered in turn.

The adaptive behavior of humans relies on the rapid monitoring of salient cues in the environment, such as threatening stimuli. People with persecutory delusions preferentially attend to threat-related stimuli R1584BABEFGJFR1584BABFAFID and preferentially recall threatening episodes R1584BABCAIIC, R1584BABCBIDA. The evolutionary advantage of a "threat-to-self" network outweighs the deleterious effects of such hypervigilant or attentionally biased threat detection. Similar biases have been demonstrated when the content of the task is concerned with threat to the subject’s self-image or self-concept R1584BABEEJBG. Preferential encoding and recall of delusion-sensitive material continually reinforces the delusional belief. Such biases appear to depend on the delusional state: that is, excessive attention is paid to threatening stimuli when the subject is actively deluded but not after recovery R1584BABFAFID.
Visual attention and early information processing in a deluded subject may be analyzed by measuring his or her visual scan path R1584BABDFHFA, R1584BABCHHJD, i.e., the direction and duration of gaze when the individual views a stimulus. When schizophrenic subjects with persecutory delusions viewed stimuli consisting of photographs of neutral, threatening, or ambiguous social scenes, they quickly identified the threatening elements of the pictures R1584BABCHHJD. However, once they identified the threat in ambiguous pictures, they spent less time than schizophrenic subjects with nonpersecutory delusions and normal subjects in reappraising potentially threatening elements of the pictures. An overly simplistic model that equates persecutory delusions with greater attention to all potentially threatening information was therefore not supported. The discrepancy between this result and the findings from experiments using single words as threat cues R1584BABEFGJFR1584BABFAFID may be explained by proposing that automatic preattentive processes are selectively tuned to threatening information in subjects with persecutory delusions; the deficient reappraisal of subtle contextual clues is a result of the jump-to-conclusions data-gathering bias we will discuss later.

Attribution theory provides a framework for understanding the causal explanations that individuals give for their own behavior and the behavior of others R1584BABHJEIJ. It has been estimated that normal people make a causal attribution (a statement that includes or implies the word "because") every few hundred words R1584BABBEBGI. Normal subjects consistently demonstrate a self-serving attributional bias in explaining the causes of events; that is, they tend to take credit for success (internal attribution of positive events: the "self-enhancing" bias) and to deny responsibility for failure (external attribution of negative events: the "self-protective" bias). Such biases may serve to enhance self-esteem.
Studies with clinical groups have typically used questionnaires that ask participants to infer the likely causes of hypothetical positive and negative events. Patients with persecutory delusions (with diagnoses of paranoid schizophrenia or delusional disorder) show an exaggeration of this self-serving attributional bias. In cross-sectional studies, they excessively attribute hypothetical positive events to internal causes (self) R1584BABGBHAF, R1584BABCFFDJ and hypothetical negative events to external causes (circumstances or other people) R1584BABFFHGD, R1584BABGBHAFR1584BABFBJIG. Deluded patients with nonpersecutory delusions do not differ from normal subjects in their attributional style R1584BABFBJIG, suggesting that the attributional bias in paranoid subjects shapes delusional content rather than form. Paranoid patients are additionally abnormal in that negative self-referent events are attributed to active malevolence on the part of the other person (external personal attribution) rather than to the play of circumstances or chance (external situational attribution) R1584BABIIHDJ.
Bentall R1584BABGDFEI has suggested that this exaggerated attributional bias functions to preserve self-esteem by limiting conscious awareness of negative aspects of the self stored in semantic memory. In this account, when such negative self-representations are primed by threatening events, external personal (other-blaming) attributions are elicited to prevent the activation of discrepancies between how the subject realistically views himself or herself ("actual-self" representations) and how the subject would like to be ("ideal-self" representations). The activation of these discrepancies would lead to a loss of self-esteem. This attributional bias occurs at the expense of contributing to the paranoid patient’s negative perceptions of the intentions of others.
This "persecutory delusion as defense" hypothesis therefore predicts that persecutory delusions will be associated with 1) normal levels of overt self-esteem (the defense’s role is to maintain self-esteem) but 2) low levels of covert self-esteem (revealed by tests capable of penetrating the defense and activating the discrepancies between the "actual self" and the "ideal self"). Cross-sectional studies examining measures of overt self-esteem largely support the hypothesis that persecutory delusions are associated with normal or high levels of self-esteem (49, 50, 53, 54) and very small discrepancies between the actual and ideal selves R1584BABBEDEB. However, a longitudinal study examining a cohort of subjects with medication-resistant delusions in a trial of cognitive behavior therapy R1584BABEIBFD suggested that the majority of those with persecutory delusions had low levels of overt self-esteem, which correlated with measures of mood and social functioning. Low self-esteem in this group therefore appeared to be best understood in terms of processes identified in depression, e.g., hopelessness or illness severity. It was interesting that the deluded subjects with the predicted normal levels of overt self-esteem had greater conviction in their delusional beliefs.
Assessing covert self-esteem is more difficult R1584BABBJFFA, and different methods for investigating discrepancies between the actual self and the ideal self have yielded conflicting results R1584BABFFHGD, R1584BABEEJBG, R1584BABECIJIR1584BABJDGHA. For example, low self-esteem is normally associated with perfectionist standards for self-evaluation, which have been found in paranoid patients R1584BABFFHGD even when not depressed R1584BABECIJI. Normal individuals more rapidly generate internal attributions for positive than for negative events, presumably because positive self-representations are more cognitively "available" to them than negative self-representations. This bias is absent in paranoid patients, suggesting that they harbor a relative excess of negative self-representations associated with low levels of covert self-esteem R1584BABDBFIE. However, a study using the pragmatic inference task (an attributional questionnaire disguised as a memory questionnaire, therefore generating measures of covert attributions) R1584BABJDGHA did not yield evidence of low covert self-esteem in paranoid patients. To our knowledge, only one study has convincingly demonstrated a discrepancy between overt and covert self-esteem R1584BABCHHAH. This research area is complicated by the fact that self-representations are likely to be multifaceted and therefore cannot readily be reduced to a single self-esteem construct R1584BABIHADE, R1584BABIHBGC.
Methodological limitations of this body of research include its cross-sectional nature, a failure to measure the dimensional severity of delusional beliefs, and a failure to examine symptom specificity, e.g., by examining the attributional style of schizophrenic patients with passivity features but without persecutory delusions. There does nevertheless appear to be convincing evidence at this stage of the externalizing personal attributional bias for negative events in subjects with persecutory delusions, but the "persecutory delusion as defense" hypothesis is less well supported. A parsimonious explanation would hold that this applies to a specific subgroup with persecutory delusions and maintained self-esteem and without a confounding comorbid depression R1584BABBJFFA. Longitudinal studies of the relationship between the attributional bias and delusional ideation are required to determine whether the bias is causal or merely an epiphenomenon. However, there is at least preliminary evidence of a residual attributional bias in patients recovered from first-episode psychosis that correlates with residual suspiciousness R1584BABCDGHH.

Both the irrationality of normal individuals R1584BABDFFEA and the subtlety of the logical reasoning deficits observed in deluded subjects R1584BABBBDEG, R1584BABBHGJC are striking. Furthermore, reasoning is heterogeneous to the extent that notions of "normative" performance are elusive R1584BABGEDFA. However, paranoid patients have demonstrated a consistent reasoning bias in one particular area: that of jumping to conclusions in tests of probabilistic reasoning.
Hemsley and Garety R1584BABGCFEC have used Bayes’s decision-making theorem as a theoretical basis for examining reasoning biases in deluded subjects. Bayesian models specify whether a subject’s confidence in a current belief should increase or decrease according to the value of new evidence received R1584BABCDCHF. The hypothesis that patients with delusions are more prone than normal individuals to draw firm conclusions based on equivocal evidence was examined in a series of studies using affectively neutral material R1584BABJDGHA, R1584BABEBFHAR1584BABCGGHA. Deluded schizophrenic subjects and patients with delusional disorder, with either persecutory or grandiose delusions, were asked to make a probabilistic reasoning judgment, namely, which jar (A or B) various sequences of black and white beads had been drawn from, when jar A contained predominantly black beads and jar B contained predominantly white beads. The deluded patients reached a conclusion on the basis of significantly fewer beads than the comparison subjects (demonstrating a jumping-to-conclusions bias) and were relatively overconfident in their decisions. However, when asked to view the same number of beads as other participants before coming to a decision, the group with delusions generated accurate hypotheses with a degree of conviction similar to those for depressed and normal subjects R1584BABEGFCE. In other studies R1584BABBDJFA, R1584BABEECHD with experimental designs using visual discrimination problems or sensible hypothesis generation and testing, deluded subjects appeared able to formulate and test hypotheses appropriately when supplied with a range of options. Taken together, these results suggest that people with delusions have a data-gathering bias (seeking less information before making a decision) rather than a probabilistic reasoning bias (deluded subjects can aggregate information appropriately, generate hypotheses, and test them).
Such a data-gathering bias was also observed during an inductive reasoning task R1584BABECFFB and more naturalistic versions of the probabilistic reasoning task in which participants were involved in reasoning about personality characteristics R1584BABJGGHJ, R1584BABCBAFH. The bias is more pronounced with emotionally salient material R1584BABJGGHJ, R1584BABCBAFH and is also observed in individuals with high levels of delusional ideation (but falling short in delusional intensity) recruited from the normal population R1584BABJDFCA.
The origins of the data-gathering bias remain uncertain. It is a consequence neither of impulsiveness in decision making nor of a tendency to use only recently acquired information R1584BABDIDJI, R1584BABDGEGI, R1584BABJCBFF, because deluded subjects are more cautious when the evidence is equivocal R1584BABEGFCE, R1584BABJGGHJ. It is not a consequence of a memory deficit, since the results are unchanged in the presence of a memory aid R1584BABEGFCE. The contribution of IQ to the data-gathering bias has not been systematically studied, but most studies have controlled for any such effects by matching participants on brief measures of intelligence.
Once they have come to a hasty conclusion, how willing are people with persecutory delusions to alter their explanatory hypotheses in the face of contradictory evidence? Deluded subjects were found to be willing to alter their hypotheses in the face of conflicting evidence from further presentations R1584BABJDGHA, R1584BABDIDJI, R1584BABCGGHA, R1584BABJGGHJ. This willingness to incorporate new information was more pronounced with emotionally salient material R1584BABJGGHJ. This finding accords with evidence from trials of cognitive behavior therapy for medication-resistant delusions R1584BABJFDGB, R1584BABGEHBE, which suggests that subjects who are able to consider the possibility, however remote, that they could be mistaken about their beliefs are likely to benefit from treatment in terms of reduced preoccupation with, or conviction in, their delusional beliefs.
The methodological limitations of this body of research are similar to the shortcomings in the research on attributional bias: its failure to measure the dimensional severity of delusional beliefs, its failure to examine symptom specificity, and its cross-sectional nature. The psychometric properties of many of the tests have not been investigated. There does nevertheless appear to be convincing evidence that hasty decision making combined with a failure to gather and assess adequate feedback may, particularly when emotionally salient material is involved, contribute to the formation of erroneous inferences and therefore to formation of persecutory delusions. We have at least preliminary evidence that there are data-gathering biases in predelusional states R1584BABJDFCA, suggesting that they may be implicated in delusion formation rather than simply maintenance of existing false beliefs.

The ability to infer the mental states (beliefs, thoughts, and intentions) of others in order to predict and explain their behavior has been conceptualized as the possession of a "mentalizing" ability or "theory of mind" R1584BABHIHJD, R1584BABCIECB. Persecutory delusions reflect false beliefs about the intentions and behavior of others that could arise from theory of mind deficits. Frith R1584BABIHDII proposed that theory of mind deficits underpin the origin of delusions of persecution, delusions of reference, delusions of misidentification, third-person auditory hallucinations, some aspects of thought disorder (disordered pragmatics), and negative symptoms (asocial behavior, blunted affect). He hypothesized that theory of mind skills in people with persecutory delusions develop normally (in contrast to individuals with autism) but are "lost" during an acute psychotic episode.
Investigations of theory of mind deficits in deluded subjects have used different methods of assessment. In what may be the first study of its kind R1584BABBHECB, a "hinting" task was used in which subjects had to infer the intentions of characters in samples of indirect speech (e.g., the phrase "It’s very cold in here" should have been taken to mean "Please shut the window"). Difficulties in performing the task correctly were observed in patients with negative symptoms, thought disorder, or persecutory delusions but not in those with passivity features or those in symptomatic remission. However, the study was flawed by a failure to match the patient and comparison groups for IQ (an important confounding variable in theory of mind tasks).
A further set of studies used so-called first- and second-order theory of mind stories R1584BABBHCGD. In a first-order story, a character has a false belief about the state of the world; in a second-order story, a character has a false belief about the belief of another character. Correct inference of the mental states of characters is required to understand the actions within the stories that are a result of false beliefs or intentional deceptions. Patients with persecutory delusions had impaired performance on questions concerning the mental states of the characters within the stories and, to a lesser degree, on reality questions (assessing their memory of the factual content of the stories). Patients with negative symptoms and those with thought disorder also showed impairment on questions regarding mental state, but this difficulty was significantly associated with more pronounced memory impairments. Patients with passivity features and those in symptomatic remission performed the tasks as well as normal subjects, indicating that any theory of mind deficit is a state rather than a trait variable. Three potential confounding variables were addressed. The findings in the deluded group could not simply be attributed to memory impairment, since a subgroup who answered the reality questions correctly still had difficulties on theory of mind tasks. Nor were the results a simple function of intellectual ability, as IQ-matched subsamples of deluded patients performed the second-order theory of mind tasks more poorly than psychiatric comparison subjects (see also reference R1584BABJEAEJ). The results were equally unlikely to be accounted for by medication effects, since the patients in remission were receiving medication equivalent to that for the deluded group. Corcoran and Frith R1584BABCJDIH therefore concluded that patients with negative symptoms or thought disorder have the most severe theory of mind deficit (first- and second-order theory of mind task difficulties, poor understanding of conversational hints, and poor knowledge of speech pragmatics) in the context of more generalized cognitive impairment. Patients with persecutory delusions have a more context-dependent mentalizing deficit (second-order theory of mind difficulties, impaired understanding of hints).
Further studies, however, have cast doubt on the primacy or specificity of such theory of mind deficits in formation and maintenance of persecutory delusions. In an examination of the understanding of "physical" or "mental state" visual jokes R1584BABFFEBF, patients with persecutory delusions showed a selective comprehension deficit regarding mental state stimuli, failing to infer the mental state of a character and thus failing to "get" the joke. However, they had no greater difficulty with this than depressed psychiatric comparison subjects. Patients with negative symptoms or thought disorder again showed the greatest mentalizing deficit and performed more poorly than the deluded group on the simpler "physical" jokes. Patients in symptomatic remission once more showed no greater difficulty with either set of jokes than normal subjects.
Some patients with chronic schizophrenia had theory of mind problems in picture-sequencing and story-telling tasks R1584BABCBFHH, in which correct sequencing in the "mental state" stories required subjects to infer false beliefs in story characters. However, there was no evidence to link this difficulty with the reality distortion symptom cluster (delusions/hallucinations). The patient group with persecutory delusions had general sequencing difficulties, and when the effects of these sequencing difficulties were partialed out, the subjects appeared to have relatively intact mentalizing. Patients with psychomotor poverty (flattened affect, social withdrawal, alogia) had deficits in both general sequencing and mentalizing.
Simplified theory of mind stories (based on reference R1584BABBDJAD) were subsequently developed to reduce the information processing load for acutely unwell psychotic patients. An acutely unwell schizophrenic group (the majority of whom had persecutory delusions) performed significantly less well than IQ-matched depressed or schizoaffective disorder patients on second-order false-belief tasks and on a metaphor task designed to assess first-order skills R1584BABGFEHJ. These differences disappeared when the clinical groups were reassessed after symptomatic remission, and this change again indicates a state rather than trait deficit. However, when the data were reanalyzed to compare subjects with schizophrenic or schizoaffective disorder who had persecutory delusions to subjects without persecutory delusions, there were no significant differences in task performance. The results suggest that the problems with theory of mind were not related to persecutory delusions per se but instead were related to the global severity and extent of symptoms (see also reference R1584BABDJIDC).
Studies using nonverbal stimuli, i.e., comic strips R1584BABHDDGGR1584BABIDEAG, driven by the hypothesis that the mentalizing deficit is specific to the disorganization symptom cluster, demonstrated that schizophrenic subjects with high levels of thought disorder had specific difficulties in attributing intentions to others, but a "paranoid" subgroup and a subgroup with low levels of thought disorder did not. However, the few subjects with persecutory delusions were inadequately characterized, in that the data were not analyzed in terms of "high" or "low" levels of persecutory delusional ideation.
Finally, using a false-belief picture-sequencing task, Langdon and Coltheart R1584BABJCDHD demonstrated selective theory of mind deficits in nonclinical subjects with high schizotypal ratings, a finding that is consistent with continuity models of psychosis proneness. The "poor mentalizer" subgroup had psychotic-like traits (ideas of reference, suspiciousness, magical thinking, unusual perceptions) as well as asocial or idiosyncratic behaviors (social anxiety, no friends, constricted affect), suggesting that defective mentalizing is linked to both reality distortion and psychomotor poverty.
The studies in the complex literature on theory of mind used differing experimental designs with uncertain psychometric properties in differing subject groups, and thus simple conclusions cannot be drawn. Groups of patients with schizophrenia certainly perform theory of mind tasks more poorly than psychiatric and nonpsychiatric comparison subjects. Any theory of mind deficit is a state rather than a trait variable. The deficit is more pronounced with second-order tasks, which tend to tax all participants to a greater degree. Performance on theory of mind tasks is clearly related to current IQ and memory, but there remains a degree of specificity of poor performance to selected schizophrenic subgroups that cannot be explained by the effect of IQ or deficits in memory alone. Patients with negative symptoms and thought disorder are consistently shown to have a theory of mind deficit, but this is in the context of more general cognitive impairment in this group. Only two studies have provided unequivocal support for a theory of mind deficit in subjects with persecutory delusions R1584BABBHECB, R1584BABBHCGD. Ultimately, while theory of mind deficits are present in some patients with persecutory delusions, they are not specific and appear more closely related to the symptom clusters of disorganization and psychomotor poverty than to reality distortion.

Few attempts have been made to characterize persecutory delusions in terms of functional imaging despite their clinical importance. Nevertheless, preliminary findings of interest have been obtained from 1) correlational imaging studies of patient groups, 2) specific imaging studies of mentalizing, attentional bias, and attributional bias in normal subjects, and 3) investigations of broader aspects of social cognition in normal and brain-damaged subjects. These will be considered in turn.
+

Correlational Studies

Correlational imaging studies have examined the correlation between regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) and either the reality distortion symptom cluster (delusions and hallucinations) R1584BABGIEGJ, R1584BABFJHFG or "delusions" as a single entity. Such "first guess" approaches are neutral with respect to the cognitive operations that these areas may subserve.
A positron emission tomography (PET) study of a group of patients with chronic schizophrenia R1584BABEHFCB demonstrated significant positive correlations between the reality distortion dimension and regional rCBF in left frontal (lateral prefrontal cortex), ventral striatal, and temporal (superior temporal gyrus, parahippocampal) areas. A PET study of unmedicated patients with schizophrenia (a mixture of never-treated and drug-free patients) R1584BABBCBBA demonstrated a significant positive correlation between the reality distortion dimension and left-sided temporal activity. A single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) study of unmedicated patients with schizophrenia (a mixture of never-treated and drug-free patients) R1584BABJDABE showed strong positive correlations between the reality distortion dimension and the left striatal area but strong negative correlations with left temporal areas. A further SPECT study of never-treated patients with schizophrenia R1584BABEADAF showed strong negative correlations between a "delusions" score and left frontal and medial temporal rCBF. While the direction of the effect thus remains unclear at this stage (in part owing to differing patient characteristics in the different studies), the correlational findings nevertheless accord with neuroanatomical models derived from data on organic lesions R1584BABJCFAF; these data have highlighted the importance of the medial temporal and ventral striatal limbic areas in delusion formation.
+

Studies of Mentalizing, Attentional Bias, and Attributional Bias

Theory of mind has been investigated by using differing experimental tasks (stories and nonverbal cartoons requiring theory of mind attributions, decisions about object function that require the modeling of another person’s knowledge) that have the common requirement of asking subjects to report on the mental states of others R1584BABBCABBR1584BABCFJJE. The results of these studies have largely concurred in identifying the medial prefrontal cortex, temporoparietal junction, and lateral inferior frontal cortex as important components of a mentalizing neural network R1584BABJJFHH. These areas are concerned with representation of mental states of the self (medial prefrontal cortex) R1584BABJIJDD, R1584BABBJEBH, representation of actions and goals of self and others (the "mirror neurons" of the lateral inferior frontal cortex) R1584BABDGGID, R1584BABIEDFG, and representation of the behavior of others (superior temporal sulcus) R1584BABFHJDB, R1584BABCHAJG.
Attentional bias has been investigated by asking subjects to determine the potential self-relevance of neutral and threatening statements R1584BABEBFBB. Attention to threatening statements deemed relevant to oneself activates a network involving the left lateral inferior frontal cortex, ventral striatum, and anterior cingulate. These areas have been previously implicated in "deep" levels of semantic processing (left lateral inferior frontal cortex) R1584BABFGDCI, R1584BABHBECC, "egocentric memory" (ventral striatum) R1584BABCGHCI, and assessing the motivational content of stimuli (anterior cingulate) R1584BABGIDEE.
Attributional bias has been studied by asking subjects to make attributional decisions about hypothetical events R1584BABEBFBB. Attribution of events in a manner that is not self-serving (that is, taking responsibility for the occurrence of negative events and considering other people or chance factors to be responsible for positive events) requires activation of the left precentral gyrus, an area involved in the executive control task of response inhibition R1584BABJIEDJ, R1584BABDBEDF. The self-serving attributional bias may be regarded as a motivationally prepotent response, and the precentral gyrus activation observed may therefore represent the activation inherent in the inhibition required to produce attributions that are not self-serving.
It is encouraging that the brain areas activated by the cognitive processes hypothesized to subserve delusion formation and maintenance overlap with those correlated with delusion scores for patient groups (F2). They have, moreover, been shown to be the site of both structural R1584BABHEAIGR1584BABJADJH and functional R1584BABHJFABR1584BABGJCFH abnormalities in paranoid schizophrenia.
+

Further Investigations of Social Cognition

Studies of social cognition during the last decade have highlighted several areas that appear to contribute to normal and abnormal formation of social beliefs R1584BABGCGJJ but have not been formally investigated in subjects with persecutory delusions; we restrict ourselves here to a consideration of the potential role of the amygdala and of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex.
The amygdala has an important role in the encoding R1584BABBGJFGR1584BABDHDHG and recall R1584BABDHDHGR1584BABEBFFA of emotionally and socially salient information (for example, from faces). It appears to be particularly important in the processing of threatening stimuli (for example, facial expressions of fear [R1584BABBGJFGR1584BABDHDHG]) and in the attribution of social meaning (for example, the trustworthiness of another person based on facial appearance [R1584BABJBFIG, R1584BABDHDHG, R1584BABEEEAA]). The ventromedial prefrontal cortex plays an equally important role in social reasoning and decision making: it appears to link perceptual representations of social stimuli with representations of their emotional and social significance R1584BABIBAJE, R1584BABEDIHC. In a complex real-life emotionally important situation, these components will operate in parallel: the amygdala will provide a quick and automatic processing bias with respect to threatening components of the situation, and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex will analyze a perceptual representation of the social situation in conjunction with a representation of the emotional and social significance of similar past events and will thus trigger an appropriate emotional state. It may be hypothesized that excessive amygdala activation may contribute to the attentional bias toward threatening stimuli found in persecutory delusions and that excessive activation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex may result in an aberrant emotional "hunch" in response to "noisy" social data, that is, a misrepresentation of body state resulting in the perception of threat where none actually exists.

The cerebral cortex can be viewed as a computational surface that creates and maintains dynamic maps of important sensorimotor and higher-level aspects of the organism and its environment, reflecting the organism’s experience R1584BABJDADA. One such abstract feature map central to the formation of the persecutory delusion would be the self-schema (an ordered map containing information about the self, relationships with others, and social situations). Its functions could potentially be modeled by a particular type of neural network, the self-organizing feature map R1584BABFIFIH. Representations in these cortical and neuronal network maps can change dynamically according to the importance and frequency of the input (experience), a feature known as neuroplasticity. General operational characteristics of these maps can be fine-tuned according to the specific processing needs of the moment, a feature known as neuromodulation.
In Spitzer’s scheme R1584BABBAIAB, acute persecutory delusions may be caused by dysfunctional neuromodulation. He noted that dysregulation of dopamine transmission occurs in schizophrenia during periods of illness exacerbation R1584BABHDDCB and that this functional dopamine excess may increase the signal-to-noise ratio in the processing of "noisy" social data, leading to the attachment of self-salience to potentially threatening environmental cues together with a reduction in activation of alternative hypotheses concerning interpretations of these environmental cues. Chronicity of these delusional ideas is accounted for by a disturbance in neuroplasticity: the abstract feature map (self-schema) incorporates the new information in line with the high degree of importance accorded to it and becomes distorted in a way that will reinforce future persecutory interpretations of "noisy" data.
While Hoffman’s pioneering computational models of schizophrenic symptoms R1584BABGIEHJR1584BABFEDHF did not specifically focus on persecutory delusions, they nevertheless posited that other positive symptoms could arise as a result of disruptions in parallel distributed processing within the neuronal network caused by breakdowns in corticocortical interactions (in this case by modeled neurodegenerative changes in the form of excessive synaptic pruning). A consequence of this excessive pruning is the emergence of functionally autonomous subpopulations of neurons, the so-called "parasitic foci." Thus, paranoid delusions of the idée fixe type could arise as a result of a parasitic focus involving narrative memory: a cortical network subserving this narrative memory could become functionally autonomous such that the same output (delusional idea) is slavishly reproduced, the reciprocal connections allowing modification by outside experience having been fatally compromised. By extending these ideas, it is possible to envisage such structural alterations contributing to the altered neuroplasticity subserving chronic persecutory delusions as envisaged by Spitzer.

+

Refining the Model

The evidence reviewed in this paper suggests that formation of persecutory social beliefs depends on 1) biased selection of threatening social data and 2) compromised inferential reasoning concerning others’ intentions toward oneself owing to impaired data gathering, a failure to accurately represent the intentions of others, and biased resolution of conflicting attributional options in favor of the motivationally prepotent self-serving bias.
Are the cognitive biases identified state factors (covarying with symptoms) or trait characteristics (present before the emergence of symptoms and therefore of possible causal importance)? The preliminary evidence we have reviewed suggests that both the attentional bias and theory of mind deficits appear to be state factors R1584BABFAFID, R1584BABDFHFA, R1584BABGFEHJ, although they may play a role in maintaining persecutory delusions once they have been established (compare with cognitive models of panic). We know of no longitudinal data on either the attributional or jumping-to-conclusions biases. However, a "state versus trait" dichotomy may be overly simplistic. Cognitive and affective processes related to formation and maintenance of delusions may vary in ways that are difficult to predict. For example, both self-esteem R1584BABEFJHD and mood variation in rapid-cycling bipolar disorder R1584BABCADHF appear to vary in a nonlinear fashion over time. Such chaotic variations may emerge from systems in which a small number of elements interact.
In future research it will be important to determine how these cognitive processes interact, cross-sectionally and longitudinally, at both cognitive psychological and functional neuroanatomical levels. Their interrelation is plausible: for example, the process of attributing causality to salient stimuli that engage attention (e.g., material deemed threatening to self) appears to be acquired developmentally early and is one of the most robust findings in causal perception R1584BABHJEIJ. High perceived levels of threat to self magnify the self-serving bias R1584BABBADIG. The ability to construct mental representations of the mental states of others constrains the kinds of causal explanations that can be made for their behavior; thus, in a group of normal subjects, theory of mind deficits specifically correlate with the external personalizing attribution bias R1584BABIBCCI. Data-gathering biases may interact with attributional bias to result in overly hasty decisions about the malign intentions of others. Exploring these interactions at the cognitive psychological and functional neuroanatomical levels will require the construction of appropriate mathematical models R1584BABBFHGA.
Of course, the individual biases identified thus far may be neither necessary nor sufficient to explain the formation and maintenance of persecutory beliefs. The rationality or otherwise of the inferential processing involved in social belief formation is constrained by conditions of limited time, knowledge, and computational capacity ("bounded rationality" R1584BABICAEE). Heuristics or rules of thumb are likely to be relied on to speed up the inference process R1584BABIBGBA. These heuristics work with the contents of the self-schema to produce a best-guess interpretation of the social world. In a developmental context, self-schemata depend on social experience over the life span, which may be impoverished by neurodevelopmental abnormalities R1584BABDGBGE, R1584BABEJFFJ or skewed by negative social experiences, such as insecure attachment R1584BABHCCGER1584BABBEFCB or racial discrimination R1584BABFEJHAR1584BABIAGFE. The best-guess output may therefore be a paranoid inference. Equally, the reconstruction of the past required by mentalizing or attributional decisions may be faulty; there are intriguing indications that conscious recollection of past events in schizophrenia is flawed R1584BABDHCBGR1584BABDAEJE. Studies of high-risk groups and longitudinal research are required to establish these associations and to determine whether abnormal social cognition precedes the onset of florid psychotic symptoms; it is interesting, for example, that in the Israeli high-risk study R1584BABGFEHB, external locus of control (a construct similar to external attributional style) in adolescence was found to predict future psychosis.
+

Informing Therapeutic Approaches

Approaches to delusion resolution that involve cognitive behavior therapy have developed in parallel to the cognitive modeling of delusion formation and maintenance over the last decade. Evidence for the efficacy of these approaches has emerged from rigorously conducted clinical trials R1584BABEFHIBR1584BABDBBCB. Therapeutic approaches that appear potentially useful from this review include highlighting the patient’s tendency to jump to conclusions, attempting to modulate the excessive attribution of negative events to people rather than to circumstances or chance R1584BABGAADF, and attempting to enhance social understanding. Trials of such treatment approaches may be nested within studies of the efficacy of the specific components of general cognitive behavior therapy for the symptoms of psychosis.
+

Elucidation at the Neurochemical and Genetic Levels

A truly comprehensive model of the persecutory delusion requires further elucidation at the neurochemical and genetic levels. Dysregulation of dopamine transmission occurs in schizophrenia during periods of illness exacerbation R1584BABHDDCB yet is not detectable during periods of remission; this pattern is analogous to the state specificity of attentional bias and theory of mind deficits. This functional dopamine excess may have a causal role in the attachment of self-salience to potentially threatening environmental cues R1584BABBAIAB, and its role in theory of mind needs to be explored. Equally, it will be important to determine which social cognitive processes are affected when antipsychotic medication leads to an attenuation of persecutory delusional beliefs. Possible approaches to investigating these sorts of questions are illustrated by functional imaging in the context of combined psychological and pharmacological challenges R1584BABHDDGF.
Finally, genetic studies, in the absence of a definitively linked genomic region of, for example, schizophrenia R1584BABBEHIC, currently are being designed to reduce genetic heterogeneity at the entry point of proband ascertainment. Thus, dimensional variables (e.g., delusional ideation, social cognitive skills) or symptom clusters (e.g., reality distortion) become intermediate phenotypes worthy of study (for preliminary examples of this approach, see references R1584BABJIDCAR1584BABEFBIB). If the relevant social cognitive mechanisms can be reliably characterized and shown to be heritable, this approach could lead to more definitive linkage results and to the elucidation of the genetic background of the illnesses in which persecutory delusions arise.
Received Dec. 28, 1999; revision received Aug. 31, 2000; accepted Sept. 11, 2000. From the Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry; and the Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Manchester, U.K. Address reprint requests to Dr. Blackwood, Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, London SE5 8AF, U.K.; n.blackwood@iop.kcl.ac.uk (e-mail ).

Anchor for JumpAnchor for JumpAnchor for Jump
+J13F1.jpeg
Figure 1.


Biases in Social Belief Formation That May Lead to Formation of Persecutory Delusions

Anchor for JumpAnchor for JumpAnchor for Jump
+J13F2.jpeg
Figure 2.


Regional Brain Activations Correlated With Delusion Scores for Psychiatric Patientsa
aOrange, left lateral prefrontal cortex. Green, left superior temporal gyrus. Pink, left ventral striatum (coronal section through head of caudate nucleus and putamen). Blue, left parahippocampal gyrus (coronal section through subthalamic nucleus).

Frith CD: The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Hove, UK, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1992
Mortimer AM, McKenna PJ: Levels of explanation: symptoms, neuropsychological deficit and morphological abnormalities in schizophrenia. Psychol Med 1994; 24:541–545
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Andreasen NC: Linking mind and brain in the study of mental illnesses: a project for a scientific psychopathology. Science1997; 275:1586– 1593
Garety PA, Hemsley DR: Characteristics of delusional experience. Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci 1987; 236:294–298
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Taylor PJ: Motives for offending amongst violent and psychotic men. Br J Psychiatry 1985; 147:491–498
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Taylor PJ, Mullen P, Wessely S: Psychosis, violence and crime, in Forensic Psychiatry: Clinical, Legal and Ethical Issues. Edited by Gunn J, Taylor PJ. Oxford, UK, Butterworth-Heinemann, 1993, pp 329–371
Link BG, Stueve A: Psychotic symptoms and the violent/illegal behavior of mental patients compared to community controls, in Violence and Mental Disorder: Developments in Risk Assessment. Edited by Monahan J, Steadman HJ. Chicago, University of Chicago Press, 1994, pp 137–159
Swanson JW, Borum R, Swartz M, Monahan J: Psychotic symptoms and disorders and the risk of violent behaviour in the community. Criminal Behaviour and Ment Health 1996; 6:309–329
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Wessely S, Buchanan A, Reed A, Cutting J, Everitt B, Garety P, Taylor PJ: Acting on delusions, I: prevalence. Br J Psychiatry 1993; 163:69–76
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Taylor PJ, Leese M, Williams D, Butwell M, Daly R, Larkin E: Mental disorder and violence: a special (high security) hospital study. Br J Psychiatry 1998; 172:218–226
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Buchanan A, Reed A, Wessely S, Garety P, Taylor P, Grubin D, Dunn G: Acting on delusions, II: the phenomenological correlates of acting on delusions. Br J Psychiatry 1993; 163:77–81
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Cheung P, Schweitzer I, Crowley K, Tuckwell V: Violence in schizophrenia: role of hallucinations and delusions. Schizophr Res 1997; 26:181–190
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Jaspers K: General Psychopathology (1912). Translated by Hoenig J, Hamilton MW. Manchester, UK, Manchester University Press, 1963
Cutting J: Principles of Psychopathology: Two Worlds—Two Minds—Two Hemispheres. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press, 1997, pp 116–125
Brett-Jones J, Garety PA, Hemsley DR: Measuring delusional experiences: a method and its application. Br J Clin Psychol 1987; 26:257–265
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Chadwick PDJ, Lowe CF: Measurement and modification of delusional beliefs. J Consult Clin Psychol 1990; 58:225–232
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Tarrier N, Beckett R, Harwood S, Baker A, Yusupoff L, Ugarteburu I: A trial of two cognitive-behavioural methods of treating drug-resistant residual psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia patients, I: outcome. Br J Psychiatry 1993; 162:524–532
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Kuipers E, Garety P, Fowler D, Dunn G, Bebbington P, Freeman D, Hadley C: London-East Anglia randomised controlled trial of cognitive-behavioural therapy for psychosis, I: effects of the treatment phase. Br J Psychiatry 1997; 171:319–327
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Tarrier N, Yusupoff L, Kinner C, McCarthy E, Gladhill A, Haddock G, Morris J: A randomised controlled trial of intense cognitive behaviour therapy for chronic schizophrenia. Br Med J 1998; 317:303–307
Jakes S, Rhodes J, Turner T: Effectiveness of cognitive therapy for delusions in routine clinical practice. Br J Psychiatry 1999; 175:331–335
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Sensky T, Turkington D, Kingdon D, Scott JL, Scott J, Siddle R, O’Carroll M, Barnes TE: A randomized controlled trial of cognitive-behavioral therapy for persistent symptoms in schizophrenia resistant to medication. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:165–172
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Kendler KS, Glazer W, Morgenstern H: Dimensions of delusional experience. Am J Psychiatry 1983; 140:466–469
[PubMed][PubMed]
Spitzer M: On defining delusions. Compr Psychiatry 1990; 31:377–397
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Chadwick PK: Borderline: A Psychological Study of Paranoia and Delusional Thinking. London, Routledge, 1992
Jones E, Watson JP: Delusion, the overvalued idea and religious beliefs: a comparative analysis of their characteristics. Br J Psychiatry 1997; 170:381–386
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Verdoux H, Maurice-Tison S, Gay B, van Os J, Salamon R, Bourgeois M: A survey of delusional ideation in primary care patients. Psychol Med 1998; 28:127–134
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Peters E, Joseph SA, Garety PA: Measurement of delusional ideation in the normal population: introducing the PDI. Schizophr Bull 1999; 25:553–576
[PubMed][PubMed]
Appelbaum PS, Robbins PC, Roth LH: Dimensional approach to delusions: comparison across types and diagnoses. Am J Psychiatry1999; 156:1938– 1943
McKenna PJ: Schizophrenia and Related Syndromes. New York, Oxford University Press, 1994, pp 171–173
McKenna PJ, Tamlyn D, Lund CE, Mortimer AM, Hammond S, Baddeley AD: Amnesic syndrome in schizophrenia. Psychol Med 1990; 20:967–972
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Saykin AJ, Shatasel DL, Gur RE: Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:124–131
[PubMed][PubMed]
Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Pliskin NH, Podd MH: Further evidence for dementia of the prefrontal type in schizophrenia? Arch Gen Psychiatry1987; 44:1008– 1014
Goldberg TE, St-Cyr JA, Weinberger DR: Assessment of procedural learning and problem solving in schizophrenic patients by Tower of Hanoi type tasks. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1990; 2:165–173
[PubMed][PubMed]
Mortimer AM, Bentham P, McKay AP, Quemada I, Clare L, Eastwood N, McKenna PJ: Delusions in schizophrenia: a phenomenological and psychological exploration. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1996; 1:289–303
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Elliott R, McKenna PJ, Robbins TW, Sahakian BI: Specific neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenic patients with preserved intellectual function. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1998; 3:45–70
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Basso MR, Nasrallah HA, Olson SC, Bornstein RA: Neuropsychological correlates of negative, disorganised and psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1998; 31:99–111
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Bentall RP: Cognitive biases and abnormal beliefs: towards a model of persecutory delusions, in The Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Edited by David AS, Cutting J. Hove, UK, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1994, pp 337–360
Fiske ST, Taylor SE: Social Cognition. New York, McGraw-Hill International, 1991
Bentall RP, Kaney S: Content specific information processing and persecutory delusions: an investigation using the emotional Stroop test. Br J Med Psychol 1989; 62:355–364
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Fear CF, Sharp H, Healy D: Cognitive processes in delusional disorders. Br J Psychiatry 1996; 168:61–67
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Leafhead KM, Young AW, Szulecka TK: Delusions demand attention. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1996; 1:5–16
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Kaney S, Wolfenden M, Dewey ME, Bentall RP: Persecutory delusions and the recall of threatening and non-threatening propositions. Br J Clin Psychol 1992; 31:85–87
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Bentall RP, Kaney S, Bowen-Jones K: Persecutory delusions and recall of threat-related, depression-related and neutral words. Cognitive Therapy and Res 1995; 19:331–343
Kinderman P: Attentional bias, persecutory delusions and the self-concept. Br J Med Psychol 1994; 67:53–66
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Phillips ML, David AS: Abnormal visual scan paths: a psychophysiological marker of delusions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1998; 29:235–245
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Phillips ML, Senior C, David AS: Abnormal processing of ambiguity in paranoid schizophrenia: a visual scan path study. Psychol Med 2000; 30:157–167
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Zullow HM, Oettingen G, Peterson C, Seligman ME: Pessimistic explanatory style in the historical record: CAVing LBJ, presidential candidates, and East versus West Berlin. Am Psychol 1988; 43:673–682
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Kaney S, Bentall RP: Persecutory delusions and attributional style. Br J Med Psychol 1989; 62:191–198
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Candido CL, Romney DM: Attributional style in paranoid vs depressed patients. Br J Med Psychol 1990; 63:355–363
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Lyon HM, Kaney S, Bentall RP: The defensive function of persecutory delusions: evidence from attribution tasks. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 164:637–646
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Sharp HM, Fear CF, Healy D: Attributional style and delusions: an investigation based on delusional content. Eur Psychiatry 1997; 12:1–7
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Kinderman P, Bentall RP: Causal attributions in paranoia and depression: internal, personal and situational attributions for negative events. J Abnorm Psychol 1997; 106:103–107
Kinderman P, Bentall RP: Self-discrepancies and persecutory delusions: evidence for a model of paranoid ideation. J Abnorm Psychol 1996; 106:106–114
Bowins B, Shugar G: Delusions and self-esteem. Can J Psychiatry 1998; 43:154–158
[PubMed][PubMed]
Freeman D, Garety P, Fowler D, Kuipers E, Dunn G, Bebbington P, Hadley C: The London-East Anglia randomized controlled trial of cognitive-behaviour therapy for psychosis, IV: self-esteem and persecutory delusions. Br J Clin Psychol 1998; 37:415–430
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Garety PA, Freeman D: Cognitive approaches to delusions: a critical review of theories and evidence. Br J Clin Psychol 1999; 38:113–154
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Bentall RP, Kaney S: Abnormalities of self representation and persecutory delusions: a test of a cognitive model of paranoia. Psychol Med1996; 26:1231– 1237
Bentall RP, Kinderman P, Bowen-Jones K: Response latencies for the causal explanations of depressed, paranoid and normal individuals: availability of self-representations. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1999; 4:107–118
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Peters E, Day S, Garety P: From preconscious to conscious processing: where does the abnormality lie in delusions? (abstract). Schizophr Res 1997; 24:120
Markus H, Nurius P: Possible selves. Am Psychol 1986; 41:954–969
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Higgins ET: Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. Psychol Rev 1987; 94:319–340
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Krstev H, Jackson H, Maude D: An investigation of attributional style in first-episode psychosis. Br J Clin Psychol 1999; 38:181–194
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Sutherland S: Irrationality: The Enemy Within. London, Constable, 1992
Evans JSBT, Newstead SE, Byrne RMJ: Human Reasoning: The Psychology of Deduction. Hove, UK, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1993
Kemp R, Chua S, McKenna P, David AS: Reasoning and delusions: an empirical study. Psychol Med 1996; 26:355–362
Simpson J, Done J, Vallee-Tourangeau F: An unreasoned approach: a critique of research on reasoning and delusions. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1998; 3:1–20
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Hemsley DR, Garety PA: Formation and maintenance of delusions: a Bayesian analysis. Br J Psychiatry 1986; 149:51–56
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Fischoff B, Beyth-Marom R: Hypothesis evaluation from a Bayesian perspective. Psychol Rev 1983; 90:239–260
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Huq SF, Garety PA, Hemsley DR: Probabilistic judgements in deluded and non-deluded subjects. Q J Exp Psychol A 1988; 40:801–812
[PubMed][PubMed]
Garety PA, Hemsley DR, Wessely S: Reasoning in deluded schizophrenic and paranoid patients: biases in performance on a probabilistic inference task. J Nerv Ment Dis 1991; 179:194–201
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Fear CF, Healy D: Probabilistic reasoning in obsessive-compulsive and delusional disorders. Psychol Med 1997; 27:199–208
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Dudley REJ, John CH, Young AW, Over DE: Normal and abnormal reasoning in people with delusions. Br J Clin Psychol 1997; 36:243–258
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Young HF, Bentall RP: Hypothesis testing in patients with persecutory delusions: comparison with depressed and normal subjects. Br J Clin Psychol 1995; 34:353–369
[PubMed][PubMed]
Bentall RP, Young HF: Sensible hypothesis testing in deluded, depressed and normal subjects. Br J Psychiatry 1996; 168:372–375
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
John CH, Dodgson G: Inductive reasoning in delusional thinking. J Ment Health 1994; 3:31–49
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Young HF, Bentall RP: Probabilistic reasoning in deluded, depressed and normal subjects: effects of task difficulty and meaningful vs non-meaningful material. Psychol Med 1997; 27:455–465
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Dudley REJ, John CH, Young AW, Over DE: The effect of self-referent material on the reasoning of people with delusions. Br J Clin Psychol 1997; 36:575–584
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Linney YM, Peters EM, Ayton P: Reasoning biases in delusion prone individuals. Br J Clin Psychol 1998; 37:285–302
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Hemsley DR: An experimental psychological model for schizophrenia, in Search for the Causes of Schizophrenia. Edited by Hafner H, Gattaz WF, Janavik W. Heidelberg, Germany, Springer-Verlag, 1987
Salzinger K: The immediacy hypothesis in a theory of schizophrenia. Nebr Symp Motiv 1984; 31:231–282
[PubMed][PubMed]
Garety P, Fowler D, Kuipers E, Freeman D, Dunn G, Bebbington P, Hadley C, Jones S: London-East Anglia randomised controlled trial of cognitive-behavioural therapy for psychosis, II: predictors of outcome. Br J Psychiatry 1997; 171:420–426
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Premack D, Woodruff G: Does the chimpanzee have a "theory of mind"? Behav Brain Sci 1978; 4:515–526
Baron-Cohen S, Leslie A, Frith U: Does the autistic child have a "theory of mind"? Cognition 1985; 21:37–46
Corcoran R, Frith CD, Mercer G: Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: investigating "theory of mind" in people with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1995; 17:5–13
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Frith C, Corcoran R: Exploring "theory of mind" in people with schizophrenia. Psychol Med 1996; 26:521–530
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Doody GA, Gotz M, Johnstone EC, Frith CD, Cunningham Owens DG: Theory of mind and psychoses. Psychol Med 1998; 28:397–405
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Corcoran R, Frith CD: Conversational conduct and the symptoms of schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1996; 1:305–318
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Corcoran R, Cahill C, Frith CD: The appreciation of visual jokes in people with schizophrenia: a study of mentalising ability. Schizophr Res 1997; 24:319–327
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Langdon R, Michie PT, Ward PB, McConaghy N, Catts SV, Coltheart M: Defective self and/or other mentalising in schizophrenia: a cognitive neuropsychological approach. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1997; 2:167–193
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Perner J, Wimmer H: "John thinks that Mary thinks that…": attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. J Exp Child Psychol 1985; 39:437–471
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Drury VM, Robinson EJ, Birchwood M: "Theory of mind" skills during an acute episode of psychosis and following recovery. Psychol Med1998; 28:1101– 1112
Murphy D: Theory of mind in a sample of men with schizophrenia detained in a special hospital: its relationship to symptom profiles and neuropsychological tests. Criminal Behaviour and Ment Health 1998; 8:13–26
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Sarfati Y, Hardy-Bayle M-C, Nadel J, Chevalier J-F, Widlocher D: Attribution of mental states to others by schizophrenic patients. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1997; 2:1–17
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Sarfati Y, Hardy-Bayle M-C, Besche C, Widlocher D: Attribution of intentions to others in people with schizophrenia: a non-verbal exploration with comic strips. Schizophr Res 1997; 25:199–209
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Sarfati Y, Hardy-Bayle M-C, Brunet E, Widlocher D: Investigating theory of mind in schizophrenia: influence of verbalisation in disorganized and non-disorganized patients. Schizophr Res 1999; 37:183–190
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Langdon R, Coltheart M: Mentalising, schizotypy and schizophrenia. Cognition 1999; 71:43–71
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Liddle PF: Schizophrenic symptoms: cognitive performance and neurological dysfunction. Psychol Med 1987; 17:49–57
[PubMed][PubMed]
Thompson PA, Meltzer HY: Positive, negative, and disorganisation factors from the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia and the Present State Examination: a three-factor solution. Br J Psychiatry 1993; 163:344–351
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Liddle PF, Friston KJ, Frith CD, Hirsch SR, Jones T, Frackowiak RSJ: Patterns of cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 1992; 160:179–186
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Kaplan RD, Szechtman H, Franco S, Szechtman B, Nahmias C, Garnett ES, List S, Cleghorn JM: Three clinical syndromes of schizophrenia in untreated subjects: relation to brain glucose activity measured by positron emission tomography (PET). Schizophr Res 1993; 11:47–54
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Ebmeier KP, Blackwood DHR, Murray C, Souza V, Walker M, Dougall N, Moffoot APR, O’Carroll RE, Goodwin GM: Single-photon emission computed tomography with 99mTc-exametazime in unmedicated schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry 1993; 33:487–495
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Sabri O, Erkwoh R, Schreckenberger M, Owega A, Sass H, Buell U: Correlation of positive symptoms exclusively to hyperperfusion or hypoperfusion of cerebral cortex in never-treated schizophrenics. Lancet1997; 349:1735– 1739
Cummings JL: Psychosis in neurologic disease—neurobiology and pathogenesis. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1992; 5:144–150
Goel V, Grafman J, Sadato N, Hallett M: Modelling other minds. Neuroreport1995; 6:1741– 1746
Fletcher PC, Happe F, Frith U, Baker SC, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RSJ, Frith CD: Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of "theory of mind" in story comprehension. Cognition 1995; 57:109–128
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Gallagher HL, Happe F, Brunswick N, Fletcher PC, Frith U, Frith CD: Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of "theory of mind" in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia 2000; 38:11–21
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Brunet E, Sarfati Y, Hardy-Bayle M-C, Decety J: A PET investigation of the attribution of intentions in a non-verbal task. Neuroimage 2000; 11:157–166
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Frith CD, Frith U: Interacting minds—a biological basis. Science1999; 286:1692– 1695
Maguire EA, Burgess N, Donnett JG, Frackowiak RSJ, Frith CD, O’Keefe J: Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network. Science 1998; 280:921–924
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM: The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialised for face perception. J Neurosci1997; 17:4302– 4311
Gallese V, Goldman A: Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends Cogn Sci 1998; 2:493–501
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Iacoboni M, Woods RP, Brass M, Bekkering H, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G: Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science1999; 286:2526– 2528
Hietanen JK, Perrett DI: Motion sensitive cells in the macaque superior temporal polysensory area, I: lack of response to the sight of the animal’s own limb movement. Exp Brain Res 1993; 93:117–128
[PubMed][PubMed]
Muller-Preuss P, Ploog D: Inhibition of auditory cortical neurons during phonation. Brain Res 1981; 215:61–76
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Blackwood NJ, Howard RJ, ffytche DH, Simmons A, Bentall RP, Murray RM: Imaging attentional and attributional bias: an fMRI approach to the paranoid delusion. Psychol Med 2000; 30:873–883
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Wagner AD, Schacter DL, Rotte M, Koutstaal W, Maril A, Dale AM, Rosen BR, Buckner RL: Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science1998; 281:1188– 1191
Symons CS, Johnson BT: The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 1997; 121:371–394
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
White NM: Mnemonic functions of the basal ganglia. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1997; 7:164–169
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA: Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain 1995; 118:279–306
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Carter CS, Mintun M, Nichols T, Cohen JD: Anterior cingulate gyrus dysfunction and selective attention deficits in schizophrenia: [15O]H2O PET study during single-trial Stroop task performance. Am J Psychiatry1997; 154:1670– 1675
Bush G, Whalen PJ, Rosen BR, Jenike MA, McInerney SC, Rauch SL: The Counting Stroop: an interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging—validation study with functional MRI. Hum Brain Mapp 1998; 6:270–282
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Bogerts B, Meertz S, Schonfelt-Bausch R: Basal ganglia and limbic system pathology in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1985; 42:784–791
[PubMed][PubMed]
Benes FM, Bird ED: An analysis of the arrangement of neurons in cingulate cortex of schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987; 44:608–616
[PubMed][PubMed]
Benes F: Neurobiological investigations in cingulate cortex of schizophrenic brain. Schizophr Bull 1993; 19:537–549
[PubMed][PubMed]
Selemon LD, Rajkowska G, Goldman-Rakic PS: Abnormally high neuronal density in the schizophrenic cortex: a morphometric analysis of prefrontal area 9 and 17. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:805–818
[PubMed][PubMed]
Heckers S: Neuropathology of schizophrenia: cortex, thalamus, basal ganglia, and neurotransmitter-specific projection systems. Schizophr Bull 1997; 23:403–421
[PubMed][PubMed]
Lawrie SM, Abukmeil SS: Brain abnormality in schizophrenia: a systematic and quantitative review of volumetric magnetic resonance imaging studies. Br J Psychiatry 1998; 172:110–120
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Goldstein JM, Goodman JM, Seidman LJ, Kennedy DN, Makris N, Lee H, Tourville J, Caviness VS, Faraone SV, Tsuang MT: Cortical abnormalities in schizophrenia identified by structural magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:537–547
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Wright IC, Ellison ZR, Sharma T, Friston KJ, Murray RM, McGuire P: Mapping of grey matter changes in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1999; 35:1–14
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Frith CD, Friston KJ, Herold S, Silbersweig D, Fletcher P, Cahill C, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RSJ, Liddle PF: Regional brain activity in chronic schizophrenic patients during performance of a verbal fluency task. Br J Psychiatry 1995; 167:343–349
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Flaum M, Nopoulos P, Watkins GL, Boles Ponto LL, Hichwa RD: Hypofrontality in schizophrenia: distributed dysfunctional circuits in neuroleptic-naive patients. Lancet1997; 349:1730– 1734
Stevens AA, Goldman-Rakic PS, Gore JC, Fulbright RK, Wexler BE: Cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia during auditory word and tone working memory demonstrated by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry1998; 55:1097– 1103
Fletcher PC, McKenna PJ, Frith CD, Grasby PM, Friston KJ, Dolan RJ: Brain activations in schizophrenia during a graded memory task studied with functional neuroimaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry1998; 55:1001– 1008
Adolphs R: Social cognition and the human brain. Trends Cogn Sci 1999; 3:469–479
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Morris JS, Frith CD, Perrett DI, Rowland D, Young AW, Calder AJ, Dolan RJ: A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature 1996; 383:812–815
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Breiter HC, Etcoff NL, Whalen PJ, Kennedy WA, Rauch SL, Buckner RL, Strauss MM, Hyman SE, Rosen BR: Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron 1996; 17:875–887
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio A: Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature 1994; 372:669–672
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Young AW, Aggleton JP, Hellawell DJ, Johnson M, Broks P, Hanley JR: Face processing impairments after amygdalotomy. Brain 1995; 118:15–24
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Kawashima R, Sugiura M, Kato T, Nakamura A, Hatano K, Ito K, Fukuda H, Kojima S, Nakamura K, Kawashima R: The human amygdala plays an important role in gaze monitoring. Brain 1999; 122:779–783
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio AR: The human amygdala in social judgement. Nature 1998; 393:470–474
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Cahill L, Haier RJ, Fallon J, Alkire MT, Tang C, Keator D, Wu J, McGaugh JL: Amygdala activity at encoding correlated with long-term, free recall of emotional information. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA1996; 93:8016– 8021
Hamann SB, Ely TD, Grafton ST, Kilts CD: Amygdala activity related to enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2:289–293
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Adolphs R, Cahill L, Schul R, Babinsky R: Impaired declarative memory for emotional material following bilateral amygdala damage in humans. Learn Mem 1997; 4:291–300
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Heberlein AS: Impaired attribution of social meanings to abstract dynamic visual patterns following damage to the amygdala. Abstracts of the Society for Neuroscience 1998; 24:1176
Damasio AR: Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York, Grosset/Putnam, 1994
Damasio AR: The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci1996; 351:1413– 1420
Spitzer M: The Mind Within the Net. Cambridge, Mass, MIT Press, 1999
Kohonen T: Self-Organizing Maps. New York, Springer-Verlag, 1995
Spitzer M: A neurocomputational approach to delusions. Compr Psychiatry 1995; 36:83–105
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Laruelle M, Abi-Dargham A, Gil R, Kegeles L, Innis R: Increased dopamine transmission in schizophrenia: relationship to illness phases. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 46:56–72
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Hoffman RE: Computer simulations of neural information processing and the schizophrenia-mania dichotomy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987; 44:178–188
[PubMed][PubMed]
Hoffman RE, Dobschka SK: Cortical pruning and the development of schizophrenia: a computer model. Schizophr Bull 1989; 15:477–490
[PubMed][PubMed]
Hoffman RE, McGlashan TH: Parallel distributed processing and the emergence of schizophrenic symptoms. Schizophr Bull 1993; 19:119–140
[PubMed][PubMed]
Kernis MH, Cornell DP, Sun CR, Berry A, Harlow T: There’s more to self-esteem than whether it is high or low: the importance of stability of self-esteem. J Pers Soc Psychol1993; 65:1190– 1204
Gottschalk A, Bauer MS, Whybrow PC: Evidence of chaotic mood variation in bipolar disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:947–959
[PubMed][PubMed]
Campbell WK, Sedikides C: Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: a meta-analytic integration. Rev Gen Psychol 1999; 3:23–43
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Kinderman P, Dunbar R, Bentall RP: Theory of mind deficits and causal attributions. Br J Psychol 1998; 89:191–204
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Horwitz B, Tagamets M-A, McIntosh AR: Neural modelling, functional brain imaging and cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 1999; 3:91–98
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Simon HA: Invariants of human behavior. Annu Rev Psychol 1990; 41:1–19
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Chase VM, Hertwig R, Gigerenzer G: Visions of rationality. Trends Cogn Sci 1998; 2:206–214
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Jones P, Rodgers B, Murray R, Marmot M: Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet1994; 344:1398– 1402
Murray RM: Neurodevelopmental schizophrenia: the rediscovery of dementia praecox. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 165(suppl 25):6–12
Dozier M, Stevenson AL, Lee SW, Velligan DI: Attachment organization and familial overinvolvement for adults with serious psychopathological disorders. Dev Psychopathol 1991; 3:475–489
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Dozier M, Lee SW: Discrepancies between self- and other-report of psychiatric symptomatology: effects of dismissing attachment strategies. Dev Psychopathol 1995; 7:217–226
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Mickelson KD, Kessler RC, Shaver PR: Adult attachment in a nationally representative sample. J Pers Soc Psychol1997; 73:1092– 1106
Cooper ML, Shaver PR, Collins NL: Attachment style, emotion regulation and adjustment in adolescence. J Pers Soc Psychol1998; 74:1380– 1397
Harrison G, Owens D, Holton A, Neilson D, Boot D: A prospective study of severe mental disorder in Afro-Caribbean patients. Psychol Med 1988; 18:643–657
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Van Os J, Castle DJ, Takei N, Der G, Murray RM: Psychotic illness in ethnic minorities: clarification from the 1991 census. Psychol Med 1996; 26:203–208
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Harrison G, Glazebrook C, Brewin J, Cantwell R; Increased incidence of psychotic disorders in migrants from the Caribbean to the United Kingdom. Psychol Med 1997; 27:799–806
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Gilvary CM, Walsh E, Samele C, Hutchinson G, Rabe-Hesketh S, Fahy T, Van Os J, Murray RM: Life events, ethnicity and perceptions of discrimination in patients with severe mental illness. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1999; 34:600–608
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Bhugra D, Mallett R, Leff J: Schizophrenia and African-Caribbeans: a conceptual model of aetiology. Int Rev Psychiatry 1999; 11:145–152
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Huron C, Danion J-M, Giacomoni F, Grangé D, Robert P, Rizzo L: Impairment of recognition memory with, but not without, conscious recollection in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry1995; 152:1737– 1742
Rizzo L, Danion J-M, van der Linden M, Grange D: Patients with schizophrenia remember that an event has occurred, but not when. Br J Psychiatry 1996; 168:427–431
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Danion J-M, Rizzo L, Bruant A: Functional mechanisms underlying impaired recognition memory and conscious awareness in patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:639–644
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Frenkel E, Kugelmass S, Nathan M, Ingraham LJ: Locus of control and mental health in adolescence and adulthood. Schizophr Bull 1995; 21:219–226
[PubMed][PubMed]
Kinderman P, Bentall RP: Attribution theory for paranoid delusions: a case study. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy 1997; 25:269–280
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Fletcher PC, Frith CD, Grasby PM, Friston KJ, Dolan RJ: Local and distributed effects of apomorphine on fronto-temporal function in acute unmedicated schizophrenia. J Neurosci1996; 16:7055– 7062
Moldin S: Genetics and mental disorders: summary of research. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:573–602
[CrossRef][CrossRef]
Brzustowicz LM, Honer WG, Chow EWC, Hogan J, Hodgkinson K, Bassett AS: Use of a quantitative trait to map a locus associated with severity of positive symptoms in familial schizophrenia to chromosome 6p. Am J Med Genet1997; 61:1388– 1396
Scourfield J, Martin N, Lewis G, McGuffin P: Heritability of social cognitive skills in children and adolescents. Br J Psychiatry 1999; 175:559–564
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]
Wickham H, Walsh C, Asherson P, Taylor C, Sigmundson T, Gill M, Owen MJ, McGuffin P, Murray R, Sham P: Familiality of symptom dimensions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2001; 47:223–232
[PubMed]
[CrossRef][PubMed][CrossRef]

No comments:

Post a Comment